大学院国際学術研究院

徳永 留美

トクナガ ルミ  (Tokunaga Rumi)

基本情報

所属
千葉大学 大学院国際学術研究院 准教授
学位
博士(工学)(立命館大学)

researchmap会員ID
6000028117

論文

 19
  • Satoshi Shioiri, Rumi Tokunaga, Ichiro Kuriki
    Cross-Linguistic Studies 41-62 2023年7月10日  査読有り
  • Ryo Takahashi, Koichi Ashida, Yasuo Kobayashi, Rumi Tokunaga, Shuhei Kodama, Norimichi Tsumura
    IEEE Computer Society Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition Workshops 3845-3851 2021年6月  
    In this study, we propose a method to estimate oxygen saturation by selecting the best bands from video images captured by a multiband camera. Oxygen saturation is one of the most important bioindicators for measuring human health. For example, when a person contracts COVID-19, which is currently prevalent, oxygen uptake does not work properly and oxygen saturation drops without the person being aware of it, which may lead to severe symptoms. Monitoring oxygen saturation is very important so that the person receives treatment before such a situation occurs. The commonly used contact sensor is uncomfortable because of its pressure and it is difficult to wear on a daily basis, so non-contact estimation of oxygen saturation is desirable. To estimate oxygen saturation using a contact sensor, the difference in the absorption coefficients of oxidized hemoglobin and deoxidized hemoglobin is used. Using the same principle, it is possible to estimate oxygen saturation without contact using the signals from two channels obtained by an RGB camera. Currently, many smartphones are equipped with infrared cameras for face recognition, and increasingly more models are equipped with multi-camera systems consisting of RGB and infrared cameras. In such cases, it is difficult to take advantage of the multiple bands because the optimal combination of bands for oxygen saturation estimation varies depending on the imaging environment and the subject. In this study, to select the optimal combination of bands from multi-band video images, we used a Monte Carlo simulation of light scattering on the skin to simulate pulse waves during oxygen saturation changes while measuring the signals with a multi-band camera. We further propose a method to select the most accurate combination for estimating the oxygen saturation based on the features obtained from the pulse wave.
  • Tsuei-Ju Tracy Hsieh, Ichiro Kuriki, I-Ping Chen, Yumiko Muto, Rumi Tokunaga, Satoshi Shioiri
    Journal of vision 20(12) 6-6 2020年11月2日  査読有り
    Previous claims of the number of color categories and corresponding basic color terms in modern Mandarin Chinese remain irreconcilable, mainly due to the shortage in objectively evaluating the basicness of color terms with statistical significance. Therefore the present study applied k-means cluster analysis to investigate native Mandarin Chinese speakers' color naming data of 330 color chips similar to those used in World Color Survey. Results confirmed that there are 11 basic color categories among modern Mandarin speakers in Taiwan, one corresponding to each basic color term. Results also showed that observers overwhelmingly agreed in their use of Mandarin color terms, including those that had yielded ambiguous results in previous studies (gray, brown, pink, and orange). There is significant cross-language similarity when comparing the distribution of color categories in the World Color Survey chart with American English and Japanese data. The motif analysis and group mutual information analysis suggest that Mandarin color terms used in Taiwan describe very similar categories and are, hence, similarly precise in communicating color information as those in Japanese and American English. These results show that three languages of fundamentally different cultures and histories have very similar basic color terms.
  • Ichiro Kuriki, Ryan Lange, Yumiko Muto, Angela M. Brown, Kazuho Fukuda, Rumi Tokunaga, Delwin T. Lindsey, Keiji Uchikawa, Satoshi Shioiri
    JOURNAL OF VISION 17(3) 1-1 2017年3月  査読有り
    Despite numerous prior studies, important questions about the Japanese color lexicon persist, particularly about the number of Japanese basic color terms and their deployment across color space. Here, 57 native Japanese speakers provided monolexemic terms for 320 chromatic and 10 achromatic Munsell color samples. Through k-means cluster analysis we revealed 16 statistically distinct Japanese chromatic categories. These included eight chromatic basic color terms (aka/red, ki/yellow, midori/green, ao/blue, pink, orange, cha/brown, and murasaki/purple) plus eight additional terms: mizu ("water'')/light blue, hada ("skin tone'')/peach, kon ("indigo'')/dark blue, matcha ("green tea'')/yellow-green, enji/maroon, oudo ("sand or mud'')/mustard, yamabuki ("globeflower'')/gold, and cream. Of these additional terms, mizu was used by 98% of informants, and emerged as a strong candidate for a 12th Japanese basic color term. Japanese and American English color-naming systems were broadly similar, except for color categories in one language (mizu, kon, teal, lavender, magenta, lime) that had no equivalent in the other. Our analysis revealed two statistically distinct Japanese motifs (or color-naming systems), which differed mainly in the extension of mizu across our color palette. Comparison of the present data with an earlier study by Uchikawa & Boynton (1987) suggests that some changes in the Japanese color lexicon have occurred over the last 30 years.
  • Alexander D. Logvinenko, Brian Funt, Hamidreza Mirzaei, Rumi Tokunaga
    PLOS ONE 10(9) e0135029 2015年9月  査読有り
    Colour constancy needs to be reconsidered in light of the limits imposed by metamer mismatching. Metamer mismatching refers to the fact that two objects reflecting metameric light under one illumination may reflect non-metameric light under a second; so two objects appearing as having the same colour under one illuminant can appear as having different colours under a second. Yet since Helmholtz, object colour has generally been believed to remain relatively constant. The deviations from colour constancy registered in experiments are usually thought to be small enough that they do not contradict the notion of colour constancy. However, it is important to determine how the deviations from colour constancy relate to the limits metamer mismatching imposes on constancy. Hence, we calculated metamer mismatching's effect for the 20 Munsell papers and 8 pairs of illuminants employed in the colour constancy study by Logvinenko and Tokunaga and found it to be so extensive that the two notions-metamer mismatching and colour constancy-must be mutually exclusive. In particular, the notion of colour constancy leads to some paradoxical phenomena such as the possibility of 20 objects having the same colour under chromatic light dispersing into a hue circle of colours under neutral light. Thus, colour constancy refers to a phenomenon, which because of metamer mismatching, simply cannot exist. Moreover, it obscures the really important visual phenomenon; namely, the alteration of object colours induced by illumination change. We show that colour is not an independent, intrinsic attribute of an object, but rather an attribute of an object/light pair, and then define a concept of material colour in terms of equivalence classes of such object/light pairs. We suggest that studying the shift in material colour under a change in illuminant will be more fruitful than pursuing colour constancy's false premise that colour is an intrinsic attribute of an object.

MISC

 53
  • 竹田 滉, 高橋 凌, 徳永 留美, 真鍋 吉仁, 津村 徳道
    情報処理学会 第82回全国大会講演論文集 2020(1) 141-142 2020年2月20日  
  • 鷹野 雅弘, 溝上 陽子, 横川 啓太, 徳永 留美, 伊藤 岳人, 山田 真希子
    Vision 31(1) 40-40 2019年1月  
  • 栗木一郎, LANGE Ryan, 武藤ゆみ子, BROWN Angela M, 福田一帆, 徳永留美, LINDSEY Delwin T, 内川惠二, 塩入諭
    Meeting of Federation of Imaging Societies (CD-ROM) 4th ROMBUNNO.1A07 2017年  
  • Rumi Tokunaga, Hirotaka Urabe, Hiroyuki Shinoda
    PERCEPTION 44 155-155 2015年8月  査読有り
  • 栗本 一郎, 大森 暢喬, 柏瀬 啓起, 松宮 一道, 徳永 留美, 塩入 諭
    生理心理学と精神生理学 33(1) 33-46 2015年4月  
    視知覚の能力を促進する視覚的注意の広がりは、空間的注意と物体随伴性注意の2つに分類される。注意の焦点を中心に広がる空間的注意とは対照的に、物体随伴性注意の広がりは注意の焦点を包含する物体内部に限られる。物体随伴性注意が、注意スポットライトの変形とスポットライトの定位における優先順位のどちらで実現されているかは、長く議論の対象になっている。本研究では、心理物理実験中に脳波(EEG)の一種である定常視覚誘発電位(SSVEP:振幅が注意の影響を受ける)を測定することにより、この問題にアプローチした。脳波計測中、参加者は物体随伴性注意を観測できるRVSP課題に加え単純な検出課題を行ない、注意の広がりを心理物理の手法で測定した。空間位置に依存して異なる時間周波数で視覚刺激を点滅させ、脳波成分と空間位置とのタグ付けを行なった。その結果、心理物理実験とSSVEP振幅の両方で物体随伴性注意の効果を確認した。同時に、テスト刺激以外の位置にランダムに呈示される妨害刺激により事象関連電位に変化が生じなかったことから、我々の研究で観測された物体随伴性注意は、優先順位によるスポットライトのシフトのような動的メカニズムではなく、静的なメカニズムに起因することを示唆していると考えられる。(著者抄録)

講演・口頭発表等

 86

共同研究・競争的資金等の研究課題

 7